Vol. 4 Issue 12 / Apr 5, 2004
Persistent Memories of Cocaine
By Jason Socrates Bardi
Several years ago, a former drug addict confided to a group of recovering
drug addicts, their families, and their friends at a 12-step program meeting
in the Midwest that he could no longer eat fast food.
In the three years that he had been clean, he said, every fast food
restaurant he entered made him crave drugs. In the day that he was using,
he would go to these restaurants, lock himself in the bathroom, and get
high. Any time he was in a fast food restaurant, the memory of those sessions
would be enough to trigger cravings.
This addict's story is common enough, and the phenomenon it illustrates
is a familiar oneaddicts suffer from prolonged vulnerability to
relapse, and the motivation to relapse arises not only because of the
physical dependence to the substance itself but because of the contributions
of memories and associations, like a fast food bathroom, that the addict
links to drug taking.
Unfortunately, these environmental and learning factors may be relevant
not only for serious addicts who have a history of abuse but also for
users who have not habituated themselves to a drug.
Recent experiments at The Scripps Research Institute suggest association-driven
desire for cocaine can persist for long periods of time even after a single
dose of the drug.
These experiments were conducted by Associate Professor Friedbert Weiss
and his colleagues, Senior Research Associate Remi Martin-Fardon and Associate
Professor Pietro Sanna in the Department of Neuropharmacology at Scripps
Research and Roberto Ciccocioppo, who is an Assistant Professor in the
Department of Pharmacological Sciences and Experimental Medicine at the
University of Cagliary in Italy and a visiting investigator at Scripps
Research.
Powerful Conditioning
The results, which are being published in next month's issue of the
journal Nature Neuroscience, are the fortuitous by-product of other
studies.
Weiss and his colleagues are interested, generally, in understanding
the brain architectures, the mechanisms, and the neurochemistry that drive
addiction and relapse. Lately, they have been asking specifically why
drug craving persists for long periods of time after an addict stops taking
a drug.
One of the issues they have focused on is the conditioning that takes
place whereby an addict associates a drug with a particular environment.
These sorts of studies normally require repeated pairing of a drug with
some environmental cue (like a particular noise) in order to determine
which parts of the brain are involved in making the association.
Recently Ciccocioppo, Martin-Fardon, and Weiss discovered that this
conditioning is powerful enough to occur following even a single exposure,
and laboratory rodents have shown conditioned motor locomotor activity
to the environment in which they previously received a single injection
of cocaine.
The scientists wanted to gauge how long this effect would last, so they
exposed their laboratory rodents to a single opportunity to intravenously
self-administer cocaine and then tested them every three months. For up
to one year, they observed a strong conditioned desire to again consume
cocaine.
The same effect was not observed when the laboratory rodents were given
sweetened condensed milka control substance that is highly desirous.
The sweetened condensed milk didn't engender the same addictive effect.
The mental processes involved in drug-related learning, says Weiss,
appear differently than those that go on in the normal learning process.
Drug-related experiences leave a much stronger and longer-lasting trace
than do stimuli conditioned to conventional rewards.
The fallout of this study is likely to be significant, since these learning
processes are important both for development of dependence and for relapse.
At the moment, Weiss and his colleagues are investigating which brain
mechanisms control this differential learning.
To read the article "Stimuli associated with a single cocaine experience
elicit longlasting cocaine-seeking" by Roberto Ciccocioppo1, Rémi
Martin-Fardon, and Friedbert Weiss see the May 2004 issue of Nature
Neuroscience or go to: http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/nn1219
Send comments to: jasonb@scripps.edu
Go back to News & Views Index
|